Tuesday, April 30, 2013

Can my son resist . . . "THE MARSHMALLOW'?



This week in lecture we have focused on the concept of "delayed gratification."  To put it simply, delayed gratification is the ability to resist immediate pleasure gain in the moment with the goal of a larger reward in the future.

One of the most famous experiments to test children's ability to delay gratification is the "Marshmallow Experiments" of Walter Mischel.  In his experiments, Mischel left children alone with a marshmallow on a plate for a certain period of time.  He told the children that if they could wait and not eat the marshmallow on the plate until he came back, then they could have two marshmallows.  A video of the experiment can be seen below:


If they ate the marshmallow on their plate before the end of their time, they would only get the one.  Mischel has found the the ability to wait for the second marshmallow in early childhood (age 3-5) predicts many benefits later in life, from better academic and professional success to lower body mass.

Because my son is 5 years old, and within the age group that Mischel has frequently tested, I thought it might be fun and interesting to have him try the Marshmallow Experiment at home.

Doing the test at home, I wasn't able to completely re-create the experiment properly, but I did my best. I placed one marshmallow on a plate and had my son go into another room (not his own room full of toys) and explained the procedure to him.  I told him I was going to leave the room for a while.  He could eat the marshmallow now, if he wanted.  But, if he could wait and not eat it until I came back, he could have two marshmallows.  I then left him alone in the room with the door closed for 15 minutes, quickly checking in once at the half-way point.

Because this test has been shown to be such a big indicator of future success in life, I was a bit nervous to see the results.  Because I didn't have access to a room with a one-way viewing mirror like the one used in the experiments, I didn't know the result until I opened the door.

When I went in, the marshmallow was still on the plate, and my son seemed very pleased with himself. After he had eaten his two marshmallows, I asked my son how he was able to resist eating the marshmallow.  I was dependent on an interview with my son after the test, since I was unable to see him during the course of the experiment.

My son said that he had resisted eating the marshmallow by moving to a different location in the room and turning away from the marshmallow so he couldn't see it.  He also reported that he distracted himself by looking at other things in the room and singing a song.  These sort of techniques for avoiding temptation are referred to my Mischel as "Strategic Allocation of Attention."

In the end, it was very "gratifying" to know that my son has learned some strategies for self-control.




Tuesday, April 23, 2013

Self-efficacy through LEGO


While the video above provides a cute bit of LEGO animation about the attributes and attainment of self-efficacy, my own parenting experience has shown that the simple act of playing and building with LEGO toys can be a powerful tool in a child's development of self-efficacy.

My oldest child is 5 years old and he is a "LEGO Maniac," to quote a popular phrase from LEGO advertising from my childhood days.  Examining my son's development in terms of Erikson's Psychosocial Stages, he falls the 3rd stage "Initiate vs. Guilt and the 4th stage, "Industry vs. Inferiority."

An important keystone in development at the 3rd, and mainly the 4th stage is the development of self-efficacy.  This is the ability of the child to take initiative and assert themselves successfully in socially acceptable ways.  The development of self-efficacy is also a vital aspect of Albert Bandura's Social Learning Theory.  In Bandura's own words:  "Persons who have a strong sense of efficacy deploy their attention and effort to the demands of the situation and are spurred by obstacles to greater effort."

Although there are many building toys that might make development of self-efficacy possible, there is something specific about the various products produced by LEGO that are particularly effective at facilitating this process.  Perhaps most important is the way that pre-designed LEGO sets provide opportunities for "Mastery Experiences."  The projects are coded for their age appropriateness and include very clear and precise directions that allow children to build complicated toys and structures almost completely by themselves.  To watch a child progress from a worried expression as they contemplate the complex structure on the front of the box, to intense concentration as they examine the directions and search for the correct parts, to pride as they examine their finished product is almost as fulfilling for a parent as the actual building is for the child.  

The use of LEGO as a tool for the development of self-efficacy has even spread into academic research, and even the LEGO corporation has developed it's own LEGO Learning Institute for the study of the interaction between play and learning.

The sense of mastery and self-efficacy that my son has learned from pre-designed sets is beginning to give him the confidence and inspiration to design and build his own creations.  Where once he would simply sit and stare at a pile of confusing and intimidating blocks, he now jumps right in and begins to build his own creations.

Saturday, April 13, 2013

Peer Learning

As I've mentioned in previous posts, my son is currently in Kindergarten at a public charter school here in Brooklyn.  This is an incredibly exciting time for all of us.  Watching our son grow and develop intellectually and socially is a very rewarding, if slightly bittersweet, part of all of our interactions.

In contrast to my time in elementary school, when Kindergarten was only a half-day, and consisted mainly of playtime and arts & crafts, my son's Kindergarten lasts all day and has a rigorous curriculum with various assignments and homework.

My son's birthday falls in October, and according the the NYC Dept of Educations rules, he is expected to start Kindergarten when he is only four years old.  My wife and I were a bit worried how our son might develop to the more rigorous curriculum he would be facing in Kindergarten.  Although he had done well in his pre-school and seemed to have the verbal and spatial skills to handle it, we were worried that he was still to immature to display the "Concrete Operational" skills that are required of today's Kindergarten students.

The "Concrete Operational Stage" is a stage in the framework of cognitive development created by Jean Piaget.  This stage is generally considered to begin around age 6, and is a stage where children begin to develop more logical thought process, especially in relation to problems of operations and conservation.

Our worries for our son seemed to be well founded at our first parent-teacher conference when it was revealed that he was performing mostly at an average to slightly below average level.  However, within the space of a month or two we found that his academic performance began to increase by leaps and bounds.

What could have been the source for this rapid improvement?  Not by coincidence, at this same time our son had formed a close friendship with one particular classmate.  This classmate was a very high performing student who had scored very highly on Gifted and Talented exams and was reading well above his grade level.  All of a sudden, it was as if potential skills that had been lying dormant for our son were finally awoken.

Our son's rapid improvement was mainly the process of "Peer Learning," or learning that occurs amongst a cohort.  In Piaget's theory, Peer Learning works by challenging a child to reconcile his mistaken thoughts and theories about the world.  When coming into contact with other children with advanced understanding that allows them to operate more effectively in the world, a child is forced to reconsider their previous misunderstandings and develop new, more accurate ones.  Piaget termed this process sociocognitive conflict.

As our son's academic skills have advanced, he has, for the most part, achieved parity with the highest performing students of his class and has developed a separate cohort with them.  Peer learning continues to be vital to his academic progress, but its nature has now changed.  Currently, his Peer Learning experience more closely resembles the theories developed by Lev Vygotsky.  In contrast with
Piaget, Vygotsky saw cognitive development as inseparable from one's social environment.  When it came to education, Vygotsky believed that "Peer Collaboration" was most important.  Rather than being a situation of teacher-student or mentor-mentee, where information is transmitted vertically, Peer Collaboration works best when children of roughly the same levels of power and knowledge work together to solve problems and explore concepts.

However, I think it would be a mistake to see the differences between Piaget's and Vygotsky's ideas of Peer Learning as oppositional, or even stages in a sequential process.  A child's full cognitive and intellectual development seems to be a fluid and dynamic equilibrium between the sort of individual striving to overcome misunderstandings and disequilibriums as Piaget describes, and social collaboration between peers to develop new and novel understandings, as described by Vygotsky.


Friday, April 5, 2013

Thinking about thinking


Our reading for this week was on Information Processing, memory and cognition.  One specific topic which piqued my interest was the concept of "metacognition," or thinking about thinking.  The skills of metacognition develop along with many other cognitive abilities, although it seems that they sometimes lag behind children's more obvious cognitive skills (memory, reading, math and problem-solving, etc.).

Because I have a young boy aged 5 who has just started kindergarten, these theories of cognition and information processing are of tremendous importance in our household.  My son has taken to school very well and has progressed rapidly with his reading and math skills.  His rapid success has made him overconfident, however.  While he has quickly learned many words by sight, and how to sound out many others, he has little patience for words he doesn't know and doesn't yet have the metacognitive ability to understand the limitations of his new skills and watch out for common errors.  As a result, he often misses important concepts of a reading assignment or understands his readings in a way completely opposite of the text.  Studies of cognitive development have shown that this ability to recognize one's own weakness and possibilities for error, and to develop strategies to check one's work are vital to a child's academic progress.  My son has not been totally without progress in this regard, however.  After several mistakes and misunderstandings, he has learned to slow down and closely read questions containing words like "except," "do not," "least," "greatest," and other words that call for detailed and close reading.

Metacognition is not solely limited to academic matters, however.  Metacognitive ability and development is vital in many basic tasks of life, such as being able to understand someone else's motivations and thoughts, or in simply realizing that reality is not always what you see.

One of the most important researchers on this topic is John H. Flavell.  One of his more interesting studies looked at young children's ability to understand the possible difference between someone's physical appearance (whether they looked nice or attractive) and their moral behavior.    My first thought after reading about this study was, "how often does a pre-schooler in our modern society really need to try and discern a stranger's morality and good or evil intentions?"  Then it occurred to me that this process most likely occurs for the first time for most children while they are watching plays or movies or reading books.  In particular, it often seems that children's interest in this type of dramatic deception occurs alongside their abilities to realize that people are not always what they seem (around age 5-6).

For my own son, this particular metacognitive development has coincided with his fascination with Star Wars.  Along with all the action and special effects, a major theme of the Star Wars films is that things, and particularly people, aren't always what they seem.  An old hermit living out in the desert is really a Jedi master.  A handsome and elegant Galactic Senator is really an evil, scheming Sith Lord.  A tiny, elderly alien living alone in a swamp is really the most powerful Jedi in the universe.  And finally, a poor farm-boy living in obscurity in the middle of nowhere is really destined to save the galaxy.





Tuesday, March 19, 2013

The Un-trained Ear




Language acquisition was the theme of our lecture this week, and the unbounded linguistic potential of infants was an important topic.  Examining broad theories of language acquisition, we first discussed Chomsky's ideas of the "Language Acquisition Device."  What he believed to be human's innate, biologically based mental structure for the acquisition of language.  This was contrasted with Bruner's theory of the Language Acquisition Support System (LASS); the idea that language develops only in a social context, guided and supported by parents and other caregivers.

One important example introduced within this theoretical clash was Janet Werker's study of phonological perception in infants (Developmental Aspects of Cross-Language Speech Perception

Werker studied infants from non-Hindi speaking families and measured their abilities to discriminate a phoneme present in Hindi but not present in English.  You can listen to the difference between these phonemes in this online Hindi lesson.  A Phoneme is the smallest unit of sound within a language that can convey meaning.  Although there are hundreds of phonemes possible, each language uses its own select and limited sample to construct its phonology.  

In both longitudinal and cross-sectional studies, Werker found that infants at ages 6-8 months were able to recognize the difference between the two sounds, but that by 8-10 months, infants from non-Hindi speaking families had lost the ability to discern the Hindi phonemes.

Theoretically, this seems to support Chomsky's ideas, as we see that infants have an innate and almost unlimited ability to listen and discern differences in phonetic sounds.  As for Bruner's Social-cultural theory, in the limited case phonological discrimination, it seems that the Language Acquisition Support System is more destructive than constructive.  The child's social surroundings reinforce only the phonological productions of their native language, and the child's broad ability to discern the entire breath of human phonology withers away.

I had more than a merely academic interest in this topic, especially since Werker's study focused on Hindi.  As I have mentioned in a previous post, my wife is from India.  Although she is most comfortable speaking English, she is fluent in Hindi as well.  I know only a few words of Hindi.  As we raise our two children here in the US, we are always mindful of how to keep them in touch with their Indian heritage.  Language, of course is integral to this process.  

Our oldest child is 5, and has already passed the "sensitive period" where he could have learned to discriminate all of the sounds of Hindi.  I'm doubtful that he heard the language regularly and often enough to learn them.  He has also just begun kindergarten, and his wholehearted commitment to English phonology in concert with his efforts to learn to read make him resistant to other sounds.  Our daughter, on the other hand, is just turning 6 months old.  She is right in the middle of the sensitive period that Werker describes, where all phonological sounds are still distinct in her un-trained ear.  Unfortunately, all the pressure is on my wife to impart this ability to her.  As I never learned Hindi in my youth, I can't discriminate the sounds, nor can I re-create them accurately.     

Saturday, March 9, 2013

Cross-race Identification Bias

Our lecture for this week on Perceptual Development had an interesting aside about a subject known as Cross-race Identification Bias.  This concept describes how people of a certain race often have a difficult time distinguishing between individuals of another race, and also have more difficulty recognizing the emotions of people of other races. 



This concept is an important part of perceptual development, particularly in terms of the development of the Fusiform Face Area (FFA) of the brain.  Human children are uniquely able to focus on and identify facial features from a very early age.  Development and utilization of the FFA is integral in this developmental capacity.

There is a downside to this process, however.  As with many other aspects of neurological development, our brain tends to become streamlined for simpler and faster processing.  When a baby grows up exclusively (or almost exclusively) seeing only faces of its own race, the FFA begins to specialize in recognizing the facial features and emotions of this race.  People are therefore able to more holistically identify features and emotions from people of their own race, but must rely on other more conscious (and fallible) mental processes to recognize features and emotions for people of other races.

Other social psychology studies have attributed this phenomenon to the In-group/Out-group effect, meaning that individuals view other people of the same race as members of their In-group and are more motivated to focus on and recognize their faces than people of another race, the Out-group. 

The concept of Cross-race Identification Bias is interesting as an academic subject, but it is of course also vitally important to many day-to-day aspects of life in our multi-racial, multi-cultural society.  Probably most important is its role in the criminal justice system and the reliance on eyewitness testimony.  This has often led to suspects being incorrectly identified by witness of other races and wrongly prosecuted and imprisoned.  African-Americans have been disproportionately affected by this tendency to racial bias in eyewitness reporting.

Speaking from my own experience as a Caucasian American, I can admit that this bias is definitely a problem for me.  I grew up in a very small and very racially homogenous town.  Today, living in a predominately African-American neighborhood, I have had several instances where I haven't recognized people that I had encountered several times.  On one occasion I actually passed a person on the street without recognizing them, and afterward realized that I hadn't even really looked at their face.

I have also often noticed myself being on the receiving end of this bias, however.  In my travels, I have often been mis-identified by locals of different countries, or not remembered by people I had talked to relatively recently.  I lived and studied in China for approximately a year and it was often obvious to me that for many Chinese, all white people looked alike.  I was often compared to many American celebrities that I didn't resemble in the slightest (even with different hair and eye color) simply because of the color of my skin.  Similar awkward situations often arise on travels to India to visit my wife's family.

More recently, my family lived in Tanzania for approximately 2 years, and local Tanzanian people would often confuse me with other white men.  Often times I would be approached by people who would begin talking as if they knew me and mentioning past conversations.  When I explained that I'd never met them or never been to that place, I could see the confusion on their face, gradually replaced by embarrassment. 

It wasn't always so harmless, however.  On one occasion I nearly had my car impounded because a parking enforcement officer insisted that I had sped off without paying for parking the day before and described an event in detail that I hadn't been there for.  It was obvious that she was confusing me with another Caucasian man from the same organization (with a very similar car), even though this man was 15 years older than me, balding and 6 inches shorter. 

 

I'm curious how this bias will play out for my son.  He is bi-racial (Caucasian and Asian Indian) and has been raised in areas where the majority of the population was black (Flatbush, Brooklyn and Tanzania).   Some studies have shown that increased exposure to other races can increase the accuracy of cross-race facial recognition. 

Thursday, March 7, 2013

Experiment On Your Kids

Now that I have your attention with my sensationalistic title, let me tell you how I did a small (and absolutely harmless, I promise) study on my infant daughter.

Our lecture for this week was on Perceptual Development, and a special part of the lecture was devoted to explaining how psychologist has learned to study infants and their development.  As the lecture describes, early concepts of infant development saw them as passive "blank slates" who did not interact with their environment in any meaningful way.  This was because infants were mostly observed and measured on child or adult standards that relied heavily on motor coordination beyond an infant's capacities.  Psychologist eventually developed ways to study infants, however, and learned of their complex sensory lives.

One important method was to carefully observe infants and measure their eye movements or looking, along with sucking rates and heart rates.  This type of observation is then paired with studies of Habituation and Dis-habituation.  Habituation refers to that fact that when repeatedly presented with the same stimuli, infants will lose interest and pay attention to it for shorter and shorter amounts of time.  Dis-habituation occurs when the infant is then presented with a new or novel stimuli.  The infants response to the new stimuli, usually measured by how long the infant pays attention, can then be compared.  For studies of visual perception, this is usually done by observing and timing the infant's gaze on various images.

For my own little study, I used my 5 month old daughter.  I presented here with a toy she wasn't familiar with (one of her brother's toys) and watched how long she stared at it.  Here's a picture of the toy:


I thought this would be a good object to use to test for her attention because it has a face shape on it, and, as we have learned this week, infants are pre-disposed to seek out human faces.  As one would commonly predict, my daughter was very interested in the toy at first, staring for 7-8 seconds.  But after several viewings, she had little interest in it and completely stopped looking.

As you might notice from looking at the toy, however, it is a spring and can stretch out to this shape:




When I showed my daughter the stretched out shape of the toy, she immediately regained interest and stared intently for 8-9 seconds.  An interesting aspect of the toy when stretched out is that the face shape becomes distorted.  It is divided into segments and stretched out.  If stretched out long enough, the face becomes unrecognizable.  It seemed that even though my daughter initially showed great interest in the stretched out shape of the toy, she seemed to habituate to it even faster than the contracted shape.  Perhaps this fact displays a novel example of infant's preference for human faces, even over novelty in shape.  Even though the novelty of the new shape was interesting, perhaps the distortion caused in the face shape when stretched out caused her to have less overall interest.

Thursday, February 28, 2013

Buddha's Brain



Our class discussion this week focused on brain development.  A particular focus of the lecture was on ways that our conscious mind (Pre-frontal cortex, PFC) can influence more basic structures and processes in our brain.  Specifically emotional responses mediated by the amygdala.  On a basic level, this utilization of PFC control over emotional function is something that everyone learns to a greater or lesser extent.  Otherwise, we all might spend our lives flopping on floors and throwing tantrums like 4-year-olds every time something didn't go our way.  More interesting to me, however, was the more extreme (and interestingly enough, scientifically measurable) control that can be developed through mental exercise, specifically Buddhist meditation.  One of the most famous researchers studying the psychological effects of meditation is Richard Davidson.  A particular focus of Davidson's research is the idea of neural "plasticity," the idea that our brain function continues to be changeable and adaptable throughout life (not just during early childhood), and that our mental activities can bring about real changes in our brain.  In his research, Davidson has often enlisted the assistance of leading Buddhist teachers; most notably His Holiness the Dalai Lama.

Although I haven't been as rigorous as I would like, I have long had an interest in Buddhism and Buddhist meditation practices.  I have attended Buddhist services and meditation instruction classes at several different Buddhist centers, and I have attempted to practice meditation on my own.  When it comes to the scientific study of Buddhism and meditative practices, however, I have somewhat mixed opinions.

On the one hand, it is encouraging to see scientific evidence for the positive benefits of meditation that I have myself experienced.  Meditation has always been an "alternative" practice here in the US, seen as foreign and strange by most people.  The growing body of scientific evidence for its benefits will hopefully lead more people to a meditative practice.

On the other hand, I fear that this "rationalization" of Buddhism and meditation is too reductive.  Although Buddhism does not believe or worship an all powerful God or creator, it is still a religion.  It is this religious sentiment, a hunger for transcendence, that attracts many to its practice.  We seek out Buddhism not only for more emotional and physical calm, but to answer the larger existential questions:  Who am I?  What is my purpose?  What happens after Death?  When meditation becomes merely a daily exercise that you do to produce some specific, physiological benefit (similar to going for a jog), it loses this religious sense of wonder.

Additionally, if we take this scientificlogic regarding meditation even further, knowing the neural pathways that are able to produce greater emotional control, more long-lasting happiness, and greater compassion and peace between all people, shouldn't we be searching for a more scientific or medical way for everyone to achieve this state?  Meditation, after all, is a diffiicult and time-consuming practice.  Many of the study subjects on meditation's neurological effects are monastics who spend hours each day, for years on end, meditating. This is hardly practical for the majority of people.  Also, there are myriad ways of meditating advocated by hundreds of different teachers and schools.  Shouldn't we instead be searching for a standardized and easily repeatable method?  An "enlightenment Pill?"

This may sound disingenuous, but it seems, to me, the most logical outcome.  But how could such "treatment" be accomplished?  Who would have access?  How much would it cost?  Would your insurance cover it?  Is spiritual suffering a pre-existing condition?

Wednesday, February 20, 2013

Is she sleeping through the night yet?


With a new baby in the house, this is a question I hear often.  How, when, and how often a new baby is sleeping seems to be a source of anxiety and frustration for so many parents.  It also seems to be a way that we judge ourselves and our parenting skills.  But even if we don't judge ourselves, there is no shortage of other people (grandparents most often) willing to do the judging for us.

In our class reading for this week (Child Psychology: Development in a Changing Society.  Ch. 6) we've read about the growth and development of the brain, and the several key factors in supporting healthy growth.  Of these factors, sleep is one of the most important.  But what is the best way for an infant or young child to sleep?  As the text points out, many cultures have widely differing ideas as to how children should sleep.  Even for adults, there are many different sleeping styles throughout the world, and even within one culture sleeping habits can change over time.

Within my own home, there is a bit of a culture clash around sleep.  As I mentioned in a previous post, I'm from the Midwest of the US, and was raised with the traditional working-class, Protestant values predominate there in my youth.  In terms of sleep habits, most people viewed their days as divided into thirds:  one third for work, one third for sleep, and one third for leisure time.  Assembly line mentalities kept people on strict schedules, and since many people were on shift work, this idea of dividing and regimenting your day was common.  Napping (unless it was Sunday afternoon and you were sitting in front of a football game on the TV) was seen as a sign of laziness, even moral weakness.  If you had a bad night's sleep, you just had to power through the next day.  I've internalized these same sleeping habits.

In contrast, my wife (who is from India) has very different attitudes and habits for sleeping.  For her, sleeping is more a matter of catching some whenever it is available.  She is a frequent napper, and seems to be able to sleep anywhere.  Additionally, any days spent with my in-laws must always have a period set aside for everyone to nap, usually leaving me as the lone person sitting awake while everyone else snores away.

Much recent research into the history of human sleep patterns is on my wife's side, it seems.  An especially interesting fact is the idea of "segmented sleep" discussed in the work of historian Roger Ekirch in his book At Days Close:  Night in Times Past.   Todays's standard of an 8 hour block of sleep is a relatively modern invention made possible by the invention of electricity and encouraged by societal changes during the Industrial Revolution.  Before these events, it was common for people to sleep in several chunks a day:  first falling asleep shortly after dusk, waking in the middle of the night for an hour or two before going back to  until sunrise, and then adding an afternoon nap as well.

While it might be impractical for adults to return to this more "natural" sleeping pattern, it is interesting to look at this phenomenon in relation to the sleep patterns of infants.  Most infants seem to naturally follow this segmented sleeping pattern, with naps during the day and nighttime divided into 2 or 3 different periods of sleep.  While most parents try to enforce the regular night of sleep on their children (sometimes gently, and sometimes not so gently), it is almost always a source of stress for both the parents and the child.  How much easier could child rearing be (for both the parents and the child) if a more segmented sleep pattern was followed by the whole family?  Not only could the baby's natural sleep rhythms be honored, but the parents could much more easily adapt to the situation, eliminating the sleep-deprived zombie state that so many new parents are forced to deal with.

A change to a schedule that more closely follows natural sleeping habits wouldn't only benefit infants.  As various studies have shown, even adolescents can have a very difficult time adapting to our contemporary "early to bed, early to rise" 8 hours of sleep tradition.  The stereotype of the sleepy, unkempt and unfocused teenager might really be the result of sleep deprivation caused by society forcing them to adapt to a school and sleep schedule that is unnatural for them.

Tuesday, February 12, 2013

Pre-natal stress and infant development


In my introductory post, I mentioned that I had two children and that I might make reference to them as I blogged about my experience studying Child Psychology.  My oldest child is a 5 year old boy, my youngest is a 4 month old girl.

Upon watching this weeks assigned video "National Geographic:  In the Womb," I was most struck by a brief passage in the video describing the effects of Pre-natal stress on many aspects of the unborn fetus' life, and felt it made an apt starting point for a small (and admittedly not terribly scientific) case study of my two children.

The effects of Pre-natal stress on pregnancy outcome and fetus health have been a common topic for psychiatric study.  A cursory search found many articles relating prenatal stress and anxiety to many changes in the development of the fetus:


Effects of maternal prenatal stress on infant outcomes: a synthesis of the literature.


Examining the correlations of prenatal stress and emotional health in my own children seemed to follow this well studied model.

During my wife's first pregnancy, she and our family were under considerable stress.  She was required to work up to 14 hour days on high stress projects until almost time of delivery, our housing was cramped and finances were unstable, and she was of course very anxious dealing with her first pregnancy.

Our oldest child had jaundice at birth, requiring hospitalization, and has always displayed signs of fussiness and anxiety.  He has always been a difficult sleeper, in infancy he was very hard to calm down when crying, and was very dependent upon having parents close and could not be left alone.  Even today, at 5 years old, he still displays many signs of anxiety, such as insomnia, frequent nightmares, fingernail biting, etc.

During my wife's 2nd pregnancy, our situation was much different.  Her work schedule was much lighter and her commute was only 5 minutes each day.  We lived in a calm and beautiful rural setting in a large house, instead of the cramped Brooklyn apartment of the first pregnancy.  Finally, much of the 3rd trimester was spent on an extended vacation, traveling to meet friends and enjoying new sites.

The difference in our 2nd child's personality is striking.  She plays happily by herself, falls asleep easily and is adept at soothing herself.  When distressed, it is relatively easy to calm her.





Tuesday, February 5, 2013

The Developmental Niche

In presenting an introduction to the various theories of child development, our course and textbook introduced the concept of The Developmental Niche.  This was a theory first published in 1986 by Charles Super and Sara Harkness ( Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 545-569).

This Developmental Niche consists of the physical settings and conditions of the child, the childrearing customs of the culture in which the child is raised, and the childrearing beliefs and goals of the parents.  All three of these aspects are described as interacting in a system to influence a child's development.

Of the three factors contributing to the Developmental Niche, it was the concept of "Childrearing beliefs and goals" that most intrigued me.  My interest was mainly sparked by my experience that parents' goals and desires for their children are so often in conflict with the other aspects of their child's Developmental Niche, as described above.

If one were to look back at our history, or even to more traditional societies today, this conflict might not be so evident.  Throughout most of human history, the various options open to a child were relatively closed.  The child of a peasant/slave would grow up to be a peasant/slave, the child of a elite/noble would grow up to be a noble.  Parents could not realistically hope for a different or "better" future for their children.  Their childrearing goals would mainly be to train their child well to properly fill the position in society they were destined to fill.

With the advent of industrial society and the breakdown of rigid class structures, however, it is now considered normal (almost compulsory) that a parent would want a "better life" for their children.  For a lower-class parent to hope for their children to grow up and join them in their same lower-class life would almost be seen as negligence.  Instead, parents are expected to raise their children to better themselves, to "move up" in society.  In our American society, our belief in the equality of opportunity and that anyone (if they work hard enough) can rise from the lowliest births to the pinnacles of our society is considered to be a natural fact beyond debate.

But how does one raise a child to advance in society and attain this bright future?  Here is where the aspirations and hopes of parents often meet the grim reality of the other two aspects that compose the developmental niche.  Not only do the physical realities of their situation limit the child's opportunities for social advancement, but parents are often completely ignorant of the skills (personal, social and professional) that their child will need to achieve these goals.

In my own life, I have experienced much the same situation.  I grew up in a Midwest, rust-belt town where my father worked at blue-collar factory jobs, and there were few other opportunities.  My parents encouraged me to attain higher education and pursue a professional career, but they had no experience with this kind of life and no idea how to impart skills that would help me succeed.  Instead, I was instilled with what they knew and grew up with:  the values and habits of a good, blue-collar worker.  I was taught the importance of being on time, taking care of my duties and not creating trouble for my peers, deference to authority, and all the other values they had been raised with.  Other vital factors such as ambition, networking and social skills, and an entrepreneurial spirit were not something that my parents or most other members of my community were familiar with.

A creative and fairly successful attempt to totally reform this Developmental Niche for under-privaledged children and help them attain greater success can be seen in the Harlem Childrens' Zone project  .  Rather than trying to simply improve education, this project attempts to improve all aspects of  a child's Developmental Niche.  Parents are taught new and more empowering childrearing and home making techniques, and entire neighborhoods are potentially transformed through their "one block at a time" method.

It's also interesting to look at the role of the Developmental Niche in maintaining social class from the opposite end of the social spectrum.  A recent study by Paul Piff (Higher social class predicts unethical behaviorexamined the prevalence of unethical behavior across social classes.  Their findings were that unethical behavior, lying and cheating were more prevalent among upper class individuals, particularly when lying or cheating would protect their wealth or social status.  One can see here how the Developmental Niche these upper class people have grown up with have pre-disposed them to see the usefulness of necessary lies when it comes to maintaining their wealth and social status.  
  


Monday, February 4, 2013

Hello,

Welcome to my blog, created for my Child Psychology Class, PSY 2301, section 1731, at The New York City College of Technology, instructor Prof. Pa Her.

This blog is created as my class Individual Project, and will be designed to meet all requirements of the assignment.

The blog will be updated weekly (approximately), and I will try to roughly correlate topics in my blog with our classroom discussion.  I plan to choose one topic or concept each week that particularly interests me and expand upon it with my own thoughts and ideas.  I will generally base it on my own experience and on my home life, as I have two young children currently at very exciting stages in their development (5 years old and 4 months old).

Examples of different topics might be:  describing how I observe certain common developmental stages playing out with my children, comparing generational differences between my own childhood and my children, and cross-cultural studies of development.  I am particularly interested in cross-cultural studies of childhood development, as I have travelled widely and both of my children are of mixed (American Caucasian and Asian Indian) heritage.

Wherever possible I will try to provide links and references to supporting websites and articles, both scholarly and from popular publications and websites.

I hope you will enjoy.